This post appeared in Dutch on the Oikocredit website.
“How are you?” It is a question we hear on a daily basis. The answer seems straightforward: “Good.” However, it is arguably one of the most complex questions someone can ask, as it requires the evaluation of every single facet of our lives: job, relationships, health, and so forth.
“How are you?” It is a question we hear on a daily basis. The answer seems straightforward: “Good.” However, it is arguably one of the most complex questions someone can ask, as it requires the evaluation of every single facet of our lives: job, relationships, health, and so forth.
Many economists and policy
makers are interested in quantitative measures of wellbeing. The concept of
wellbeing is meant to represent the quality of life of an individual or group. In
a way, it puts a number on the answer to “how are you?” And just like this
question is difficult to answer, the measurement of wellbeing is difficult to
do.
Beyond
GDP
For a long time, economists
have used monetary measures to proxy wellbeing, such as the gross domestic
product (GDP). They are – at least in theory - relatively easy to calculate as
the components can be counted and added up. But, as a sign in Einstein’s office
at Princeton read: “Not everything that counts can be counted, and not
everything that can be counted counts.”
Wellbeing is influenced by
factors that are difficult or impossible to count, such as the quality of
relationships or the way you feel when you wake up in the morning. Economists
are therefore increasingly interested in alternative measures of wellbeing,
such as happiness.
It’s
all about the money?
As part of my PhD research
I travelled through Bolivia and interviewed the local population about their happiness.
One of my main research questions was how happiness is associated with other
aspects of life. For example, does money make you happy? Based on these data I
collected, I can answer sí wholeheartedly.
Richer people were happier and many other studies have come to the same
conclusion.
However, already in the
1970s, Richard Easterlin found that the relationship between income and
happiness is more complicated than it seems. He found that richer people within a country were indeed happier,
but that richer countries are not necessarily happier on average. This “Easterlin paradox” can be
explained by the fact that individuals tend to compare themselves to others. In
that sense, it is not the absolute income that matters, but the relative
income. For example, you might be content with your own car until your
neighbour buys a new, slightly bigger, one.
Does
it count?
Why is this kind of
knowledge important? First of all, a wider view of wellbeing can help to set
priorities for policy. If we assume that relative income is more important than
absolute income, we should aim to reduce income inequality. After all, larger
income differences would then cause larger aggregate unhappiness.
Alternative measures of wellbeing
can also help us to see things we would otherwise miss. A nice example is
Egypt. In the run-up to the Arab Spring, the GDP per capita was steadily
growing; the country was doing well according to the traditional measures.
However, the percentage
of happy people in 2010 was only half of what it was five years earlier. The discontent
would come to an explosion at the Tahrir Square at the beginning of 2011.
It would be good if
economists and policy makers looked beyond the traditional approaches to
measure wellbeing. There are many more possibilities to count what really
counts.
Read
more?
- The World Happiness Reports of 2012 and 2013 offer a good introduction to anyone who is interested in happiness research.
- An article by Betsy Stevenson and Justin Wolfers, who investigate the Easterlin Paradox using new data. According to their results richer countries are indeed happier than their poorer counterparts, which contrasts with Easterlin’s findings.
No comments:
Post a Comment